1)中国科学院心理研究所,认知科学与心理健康全国重点实验室,北京 100101;2)中国科学院大学心理学系,北京 100049
Tel:
科技部科技创新2030-“脑科学与类脑研究”重大项目(2022ZD0205100,2021ZD0203800)和国家自然科学基金(32371106,32430043)资助。
1)State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Science and Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China;2)Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, China
This work was supported by grants from the STI2030-Major Project (2022ZD0205100, 2021ZD0203800) and The National Natural Science Foundation of China (32371106, 32430043).
自我面孔是自我社会性符号的代表。作为自我意识的窗口,自我面孔相比于他人面孔享有一系列加工优势,包括识别优势、注意优势、积极优势。识别优势是指对自我面孔的更快更准确地识别,这一优势不受面孔朝向和空间频率成分的影响,并得到早期脑电成分如N170的支持。注意优势体现为自我面孔具有强势的注意捕获和保持能力。积极优势进一步表征了自我面孔加工优势,个体倾向于认为自己的面孔比客观表征更具吸引力或更值得信赖。自我面孔优势效应可以在无意识水平发生,并受到个体自尊、文化差异、感觉信息整合等多种因素的调节。在个体自尊层面,高自尊个体通常在面对干扰时也能保持对自我面孔的各类优势加工。而低自尊个体对自我面孔的识别优势则容易受到社会线索的干扰,对自我面孔的注意加工也会受到任务类型的影响。在文化差异层面,相比东亚集体主义文化下的群体,强调独立自我构建的西方文化下的群体表现出更强的自我面孔优势。感觉信息整合也会影响自我面孔优势加工,例如,同步的视觉-触觉信息能增强自我面孔优势。在神经机制层面,自我面孔优势加工在整体上表现出右脑偏侧化特性,并涉及枕叶、颞叶、额叶、脑岛、扣带回等多个脑区。近年来,研究者已围绕自我面孔的加工及其优势效应提出多个理论阐述,然而也有一些研究发现,自我面孔与熟悉面孔在行为和神经层面并不存在明显优势差异,使得自我面孔特异性仍存在争议。进一步论证自我面孔特异性的关键在于采用面孔联结范式来控制熟悉性,从而厘清自我面孔与熟悉面孔之间是否存在本质差异。由于自我面孔加工优势效应与社会认知障碍(例如,孤独症谱系障碍、精神分裂症)存在紧密联系,明确自我面孔特异性有潜力为这些社会认知障碍的早期识别、分级和后期干预提供新的关键线索,不仅具有理论意义、还具有重要社会价值。
Self-face is a unique and highly distinctive stimulus, not shared with others, and serves as a reliable marker of self-awareness. Compared to other faces, self-face processing exhibits several advantages, including the self-face recognition advantage, self-face attention advantage, and self-face positive processing advantage. The self-face recognition advantage manifests as faster and more accurate identification across different orientations and spatial frequency components, supported by enhanced early event-related potential (ERP) components, such as N170. Attentional biases toward self-face are evident in target detection during spatial tasks and the attentional blink effect in temporal paradigms. However, measurement sensitivity, perceptual load, and task demands contribute to some mixed findings. Positive biases further characterize the self-face processing advantage, with individuals perceiving their faces as more attractive or trustworthy than objective representations. These biases even extend to self-similar others, influencing social behaviors such as trust and voting preferences. Self-face processing advantages have been observed at an unconscious level and are regulated by several factors, including self-esteem, cultural differences, and multisensory integration. Cultural and individual differences play a crucial role in shaping self-face advantages. Individuals from Western cultures, which emphasize independent self-construal, exhibit stronger self-face biases compared to those from East Asian collectivist contexts. Self-esteem also modulates self-face advantages: high-self-esteem individuals generally maintain their self-face recognition advantage despite interference, exhibit attentional prioritization of self-faces, and demonstrate enhanced positive associations with subliminal self-faces. In contrast, low-self-esteem individuals display recognition vulnerabilities to social cues, show context-dependent attentional divergence (prioritizing others’ faces in task-oriented settings while prioritizing self-face in free-viewing tasks), and exhibit reversed positive associations with subliminal self-faces. Multisensory integration, such as synchronized visual-tactile cues, enhances self-face advantages and induces perceptual plasticity. This phenomenon is exemplified by the enfacement illusion, in which synchronous visual and tactile inputs update the mental representation of the self-face, leading to assimilation with another face. Neuroanatomically, self-face processing is predominantly lateralized to the right hemisphere and involves a network of brain regions, including the occipital lobe, temporal lobe, frontal lobe, insula, and cingulate gyrus. Disruptions in these networks are linked to self-face processing deficits in socio-cognitive disorders. For instance, autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and schizophrenia are associated with attenuated self-face advantages and abnormal neural activity in regions such as the right inferior frontal gyrus, insula, and posterior cingulate cortex. These findings suggest that self-face processing could serve as a potential biomarker for the early diagnosis and intervention of such disorders. In recent years, researchers have proposed various theoretical explanations for self-face processing and its advantage effects. However, some studies have reported no significant behavioral or neural advantages of self-faces over familiar faces, leaving the specificity of self-face a subject of debate. Further elucidation of self-face specificity requires the adoption of a face association paradigm, which controls for facial familiarity and helps determine whether qualitative differences exist between self-faces and familiar faces. Given the close relationship between self-face processing advantages and socio-cognitive disorders (e.g., ASD, schizophrenia), a deeper understanding of self-face specificity has the potential to provide critical insights into the early identification, classification, and intervention of these disorders. This research holds both theoretical significance and substantial social value.
唐小下,张姝佳,张颖,王莉.自我面孔优势加工及其机制[J].生物化学与生物物理进展,2025,52(7):1771-1791
复制
生物化学与生物物理进展 ® 2025 版权所有 ICP:京ICP备05023138号-1 京公网安备 11010502031771号